The Christchurch Civic Creche Case

News Reports Index

1995




Butterworths Family Law Journal. 1(11):251-258; Sep 1995.

Child witnesses in sexual assault cases — Issues of credibility and interviewing practices
by Wendy Ball, Lecturer in Law, University of Waikato.

Comments on the provisions of the Evidence Amendment Act 1989 which allows videotaped evidential interviews with children, and notes the 2 common challenges to such testimony: the credibility of the child and of the interviewing techniques used. Looks at issues of contamination of memory and contamination of consciousness, focusing on the use of anatomically correct dolls. Outlines UK and Canadian approaches to interviewing child complainants and discusses attitudes to the reliability of children as witnesses


New Zealand has recently been the reluctant host of what appears to be an upsurge of cases involving child sexual abuse. These have been not only those involving family, extended family and "strangers", but also cases involving preschool, day care or kindergarten multiple offending. This increase in prosecuted cases could well be attributable in part to recent amendments to the Evidence Act of 1908 which now allows for child complainants to present their evidence-in-chief by way of videotaped evidential interview and thus provides a less threatening mode for children to present their complaints. In nearly all these cases there has been a focus on two aspects of the testimony of children : that of their credibility and that of the interviewing techniques employed to gain videotaped evidence of their complaint/s.

There are two approaches that will be commonly taken in defending cases of child assault and abuse. The first will challenge the validity of the competency test in the videotaped interview with the child complainant as required under reg 5(1)(c) and s 23E(2) of the Evidence Amendment Act 1989. The second will challenge the manner in which that interview was taken and the interviewing practices of those in charge.

Both these areas need to be examined for they both affect the overall validity of the charge of which they are the centre. The interview deals with the child complainant in a setting other than the traditional courtroom venue, and as such is open to vigorous check, and cross examination, and criticism. The interviewing practices therefore are a major lynchpin of both prosecution and defence cases.


Interviewing Child Complainants

In New Zealand "Interviewers" are now those persons trained and employed by the New Zealand Children & Young Persons Service, Specialists Services Division. These persons have been accepted by the Courts as expert in the specialised area of interviewing children for both social and Court intervention.

The interviewers of children in what are termed "evidential interviews"are those whose skills fall within the category of what is called "technical expertise", and this expertise can be the focus of both prosecution and defence cases, particularly as the law now requires neither corroboration of children's evidence, nor an open caution in the absence of corroboration.

In R v H (1993) [1] Heron J outlines the importance of evidential interviews and the role of the interviewer at pp 5-6:

I cannot overlook the considerable evidential advantage which the legislature has conferred on the prosecution by this procedure of evidence in chief by video. It presents the jury with the evidence which subject only to cross examination and re-examination remains in that form. It can never be changed. In my view every care should be taken to limit the number and extent of diagnostic interviews once sexual abuse is either gravely suspected or has been expressly referred to. I understand in other cases the prosecution has relied at all times on evidential interviews where the formalities required by the Regulations have been observed in each case and diagnostic interviews not on video have not intruded into the process as has happened here. There is firstly the promise to tell the truth, which in itself is a safe-guard and a reminder to the child of the need for the appropriate solemnity.

Further the video is available to the parties and the jury to see the manner and style of interviewing and the way in which the revelations occurred and their intrinsic credibility. The sexual abuse guidelines themselves emphasise the techniques of interviewing, both in respect of evidential interviews and diagnostic interviews.

The Evidence Act does not specify the way in which a videotape is to be prepared, or the way in which the interviews recorded are to be conducted. The Regulations give the rules pursuant to s 23I of the Evidence Act, but nowhere within the Act or Regulations are the qualifications and skills necessary for interviewers specified. There is a general presumption that the interviewer has the ability to judge whether the child complainant has the requisite understanding of truth, lies and a promise as stated in reg 5(1)(c).

Hardie Boys J stated in R v Seth (1992) [2]

The Court must recognise the underlying assumption that the interviewer will be sufficiently trained and competent not to proceed further unless she [the interviewer] makes the requisite determination.

In the United Kingdom the degree of the skills and expertise necessary for interviewers of children in alleged abuse situations was recognised in the Report of Inquiry into Child Abuse in Cleveland 1987, which included some guidelines set down and agreed to by all professionals involved in the Report. These stated guidelines are: [3]

1.              The undesireability of calling them "disclosure" interviews, which precluded the notion that sexual abuse might not have occurred.

2.              All interviews should be undertaken only by those with some training, experience and aptitude for talking with children.

3.              The need to approach each interview with an open mind.

4.              The style of the interview should be open-ended questions to support and encourage the child in free recall.

5.              There should be where possible only one, and not more than two, interviews for the purpose of evaluation, and the interview should not be too long.

6.              The interview should go at the pace of the child and not of the adult.

7.              The setting for the interview must be suitable and sympathetic.

8.              It must be accepted at the end of the interview the child may have given no information to support the suspicion of sexual abuse and the position will remain unclear.

9.              There must be careful recording of the interview and what the child says, whether or not there is a video recording.

10.          It must be recognised that the use of facilitative techniques may create difficulties in subsequent Court proceedings.

11.          The great importance of adequate training for all those engaged in this work.

12.          In certain circumstances it may be appropriate to use the special skills of a "facilitated" [4] interview. That type of interview should be treated as a second stage. The interviewer must be conscious of the limitations and strengths of the techniques employed. In such cases the interview should only be conducted by those with special skills and specific training.

In summary, it is the interviewing practices which come into question and lead to the interviewers being in a vulnerable position when having to demonstrate that the requirements of the Evidence Act 1908 have been met. The interviews can be of two types:

·                Evidential

   This interview is intended to form the child complainant's evidence-in-chief in accordance with the Evidence Amendment Act 1989 and its Regulations 1990, with the child and interviewer being liable to cross examination and re-examination.

·                Diagnostic

            This type of interview is not intended to form part of the Court proceedings, and as such is not normally videotaped. [5] The purpose of this interview is to ascertain whether or not there is some type of problem with the child or the behaviour of the child that necessitates further long term therapeutic intervention. During the course of the interview should there be disclosure of any abuse an evidential interview will be done separately. The interviewer or therapist is unlikely to be called to give evidence although may be asked to appear at the Court proceedings to be of assistance to the Judge in deciding the appropriate manner or mode of the child giving testimony: s 23D(3).

In the evidential interviews, the interviews and the process of the interviews are liable to the defence strategy of challenge by way of "contamination" theories. This goes to the credibility of both the child [and the interviewer]. Obviously the focus is the contamination of the child's testimony; however I believe that tacitly the interviewer is also a focus for this.



Contamination Defence

Contamination of Memory

The theory of contamination or "pollution" of the memory of children is based on the premise that repeated interviews and the use of leading questions by official interviewers can create a role play or "rote" learning situation so that children re-enact and repeat facts which then can form the basis of false allegations. Parents, from feelings of powerlessness and guilt may often perpetuate this cycle by their use of leading questions.

The Australian case of "Mr Bubbles" highlights this type of contamination of memory that can be levelled by the defence at interviewers and those involved in prosecution. Mr and Mrs Deren were charged with the sexual assault of a number of children, all under the age of six, who attended a suburban kindergarten. The allegations were investigated by police and a number of interviews were carried out by a police officer with very little experience in interviewing young children. The allegations made by the police, as a result of the investigation, involved such acts as the videotaping of children in pornographic poses, taking children from the kindergarten property and various sexual acts of indecency. All charges failed at the depositions hearing stage, and because of this criticism was levelled at the police interviewers. This criticism involved both their interviewing practices and their directions to parents of complainant children.

As Greg Walsh stated: [6]

The investigative techniques relied upon by the authorities involved a number of repetitive interviews and the use of leading questions of the children. The case illustrates the way in which a child's memory can be contaminated [by the interviewer].



Contamination of Consciousness

The theory of contamination or "pollution" of consciousness is based on the premise that a child's conscious reality can be affected by external stimuli in combination with their natural desire to please. Non verbal prompts to assist in accurate and detailed disclosures are being used more frequently; [7] such tools as context reinstatement (taking the child back into the situation), toys in the interviewing room that have been brought in from the place where the alleged offence occurred; scale models etc.

The most controversial non verbal prompt which has often been the focus of defence strategy, is the use of anatomically correct or detailed dolls.It is alleged that a child who is in a strange and tense environment such as an interview setting, who sees a doll with prominent genitalia will unconsciously pick up messages that a way to please is to play with this doll, and in particular to focus on the obvious genitalia of the doll. This may then lead the interviewer to believe the child has been sexually abused which may subsequently lead to false allegations in a Court case.

Again Greg Walsh offers comment:

These dolls are often used in the questioning of very young children. The dolls are generally not anatomically correct or even anatomically explicit. They are generally sexually graphic. As to the use of these dolls, Christiansen [a US Psychologist] says: "But what if the child has not been abused? Under these circumstances the interview can be an exercise in learning, not recall. Here is this person, the interviewer, who wants something from him. His mother or father wants something from him as well. They want him to say something, to tell them about something. The child is bound to try to figure out what they are after, especially since it is clear that he gets a positive reaction from them when he says certain things. If he can determine what they want him to say, they will be happy and love him. So he listens to their questions and tries to sort it out. Playing with dolls in certain ways also gets a good reaction. The child may even determine that they want to tell a certain kind of story, and he invents one. They love him for it." (The Testimony of Child Witnesses: Fact, Fantasy and the Influence of Pre-Trial Interviews [1987] 62 Washington LR 712-13) [8]

There are many opponents to the use of anatomically correct or detailed dolls within the interview stage, as it, in the views expressed, increases the chances of contamination: [9]

... No matter what they are called [correct or detailed], they are a serious contamination to any meaningful psychiatric examination. Unless the child has been previously evaluated by one of these "validators", it is most likely that the child has never seen such a doll before. The child cannot but be startled and amazed by such a doll.

... The likelihood of a child ignoring these unusual genital features is almost at a zero level. Accordingly, the dolls demand attention and predictably will bring about the child talking about sexual issues.

... Again, contamination here is so great that the likelihood of differentiating between bona fide and fabricated sex abuse has become reduced considerably by the use of these terrible contaminants.

I would however question the validity of these criticisms when applied to the New Zealand situation. Caution is and has been exercised by interviewers in New Zealand in that these dolls are never out "on display" but are kept out of sight of child until the child or the interviewer deems it necessary to retrieve them. Usually a body chart rather than the dolls is used to define words for body parts, not the dolls and mere non verbal language is not taken in isolation; there must be reasonable parity between the questions being asked of the child, the verbal answers and the non verbal body language involving the "doll" before any conclusions are tentatively arrived at. Certainly the "contaminants" are treated with respect and used correctly.

The current Draft Sexual Abuse Guidelines for Evidential Interviews developed in conjunction with the Police and the New Zealand Children and Young Persons Specialist Department state in paragraph (n): [10]

...Do not use anatomically correct dolls unless the child is totally unable to describe the abuse. Do not introduce the dolls until specific sexual abuse has been mentioned. The dolls can then be used to clarify body parts and also what happened, eg where people were sitting, touching, etc.

Dr Karen Zelas, New Zealand Specialist Child Psychiatrist, also advocates caution in the use of anatomically correct dolls in interview situations because of the comments of those against their use. In the case of R v Ellis (1993) [11] she stated as an expert witness under s 23G:

However because of the quite commonly held but uniformed view that they [anatomically correct dolls] are suggestive I do not encourage interviewers to use anatomically correct dolls in the first instance with a child but to use them after a child has made a disclosure of abuse either in the interview or where a child is then not giving that same information in the interview or where it is clear that it is going to assist the child to describe or demonstrate something which has occurred once the child has indicated that it has occurred.

Certainly any use of non verbal prompts must be subject to caution and common sense on the part of interviewers. Ongoing awareness is needed that defence strategies used in cross examination of scrutinising interviewing techniques of these type are well within the legal tenet of protection of both the child and the accused from any injustice.


Ideological Contamination

Yet another type of contamination is ideological. This is described by Goode:

The risks of inadvertent memory contamination are exponentially increased where there is the involvement of an ideologically committed "professional" or "expert" in the legal process. [12]

In these cases these experts presume the guilt of the alleged offender, become champions of "a cause" and thus lose all perspective and objectivity. This of course goes against the basic tenet in law, of a person being presumed innocent until proven guilty, and this zealousness can of course create pressure for a child in an interview situation. Disbelievers of the occurrence and reality of child sexual abuse would claim this form of contamination for nearly all cases. However given the current research and learning in this field there are now many safeguards for the interviewers and experts to ensure this zeal does not go unnoticed and unfettered, eg most interviewers now have independent supervision of all their work to ensure the non contamination of their working practices. When giving evidence in R v Ellis (1993) [13] Dr Karen Zelas explained to the Court the concept of supervision and its necessity for interviewers working with abused children:

It involves meeting either as a group of interviewers with me or from time to time individually with me and discussing aspects of their work, looking at pieces of videotaped interviews they have done, particularly with regard to technique or if they found a child difficult to communicate with looking at how that might have been achieved more effectively. On occasion looking at something and helping them to appraise what steps, if any, should be recommended on the basis of that communication from the child.

... It includes more general discussion of matters to do with sexual abuse or to do with something the interviewers or I have been reading, journal articles etc.... Well, supervision as I have described it to you has a training element in it.




United Kingdom Approach to Interviewing Child Complainants

Glanville Williams [14] commented on his ideals of interviewers during the United Kingdom debates leading to the enactment of the Criminal Justice Act 1990. He stated:

The child examiner should have professional training; she should be chosen for intelligence, and should be acquainted with the various influences and motives that may cause a child to give mistaken or otherwise false evidence. She may, in fact, have better knowledge of these matters, and better insight into the mind of a child, than a lawyer, particularly one who has little experience of children. On the whole, I do not think that lawyers have just cause for complaint if their functions are partly transferred, in these exceptional cases, to another kind of professional. Lawyers do not take part in investigations by the police, why should they object to investigations by psychologists?

Glanville Williams offers also his comments as to who are the most appropriate people to be "child examiners" or interviewers in cases of child abuse:

... several countries have child examiners, and the determination of qualities needed for this appointment is a straightforward matter. Preferably the examiner would be a child psychologist (and best of all a woman child psychologist, if the complainant is a girl). But she may (in descending order of preference) be a psychiatrist, a police surgeon, or at a last resort a social worker highly qualified by experience and intelligence. [15]

In the United Kingdom, the Home Office published a Memorandum of Good Practice [16] for the videotaped interviews of child complainants which has been received by social workers, psychologists and psychiatrists with mixed reactions. Generally the Memorandum was thought to be helpful and instructive, however as to whether the document achieved the finite guidelines for carrying out interviews considered necessary and wanted by the interviewers is another question. It did not tell them how to help the children differentiate between truth and lies, or how to ensure they appreciate the solemnity of the legal promise.

It did however address such things as "timing", nature of questioning, and how to conclude interviews, and even went to the extent of suggesting that one hour is the maximum duration for a disclosure or evidentiary interview and that any exceptions to this should be rare and justifiable.

Indeed the guidelines state four phases the interview is to be divided into: (i) rapport- building; (ii) free narrative at the child's pace; (iii) more active questioning (again subdivided into four phases with kinds of questions to be employed); (iv) closing the interview. To encompass the four phases within one hour optimum seems unlikely. The Home Office team were obviously reluctant to in any way, implied or direct, give favour to a multiplicity of interviews for whatever reason. It appears that there is a fear of coaching the child should there be more than one or two interviews, thus opening the portals of "contamination" to the defence. However as McEwan states:

Investigators are considerably handicapped by the requirement that all relevant information is provided by the child at one fell swoop.

Further,

There is clear conflict here between the sort of interview required for investigative purposes and the function of compiled evidence-in-chief ready for Court. [17]

Gerrilyn Smith [18] succinctly takes this argument further on behalf of the interviewer who is, within the one to two hour limit, dealing with a child allegedly having suffered more than one episode of sexual abuse and with this fear of contamination:

There is often a fear expressed by the Crown Prosecution that any further discussion of sexual abuse by the child or their carers will somehow taint the evidence.

...The misapprehension that the need to treat the child may conflict with the needs of justice is in part based on the belief that the original disclosure represents the totality of the child's experience.

... It also fails to recognise that unless traumatic material can be discussed in a safe environment, it will continue to trigger flashbacks and strong emotional reactions. A child or young person is likely to give better evidence if they have had the opportunity to talk about their experience of sexual abuse and have been prepared specifically for the Court appearance. [19]

The Memorandum also recommends that direct or leading questions not be used by interviewers. This would appear to make sense in terms of the evidence-in-chief requirement, but not in terms of child development and the unease with which children relate sensitive issues to a "stranger". The expertise necessarily required of these specialist interviewers is increased by the publication of this Memorandum.



Canadian Approach to Interviewing Child Complainants

One method for interviewing child witnesses that is being trialed in British Columbia is that called The Step-Wise Method. This method is intended to provide an organised step approach to interviews to maximise recall while minimising contamination.

These steps begin with the most open, least leading form of questioning and proceed to more specific forms of questioning as circumstances require. The initial goal is to give the child every opportunity to provide free narrative before other forms of questioning are used. The next step is to use open, general questions to prompt more recall without leading the child in any way. Following this, the child is prompted for more specific recall, but only by requesting elaboration on details already described or introduced in the child's earlier free narrative report. [20]

This model is to be used in accordance with the goals of an effective interview : (1) minimising the trauma of investigation for the child; (2) maximising the information obtained from the child about the alleged event(s); (3) minimising the contaminating effects of the interview on the child's memory of the events; and (4) maintaining the integrity of the investigative process.



The Interview Process

In researching this paper, I have found a dearth of material which directly relates to interviewing techniques as such; the information mostly relates to credibility or reliability of child witnesses, not to the actual task or skill of interviewing.

This would seem to imply that interviewing, is seen as a simple straightforward matter, not involving great skill. However, as Spencer states: [21]

Interviewing, often described as a "conversation with a purpose", is not a natural or innate ability, nor is it easy as this definition implies.

... Conducting interviews with child sexual abuse witnesses must rank as one of the most demanding interview situations, due to the sensitivity of the topic, the reticence of victims, and the potential conflict between evidentiary and therapeutic goals.

Spencer and Flin detail studies made of effective and non effective interviewing techniques or styles and emphasise several points, one main point being the need for specialist training for interviewers:

In a more recent study of real criminal investigations, Aldridge and Cameron (1989) recorded and analysed videotapes of interviews with alleged child sexual abuse victims, and concluded that, "Without training, experienced police officers and social workers perform inadequately on a wide range of interview behaviours". [22]

The authors make another salient point about the striking similarity between ineffective interview questions which are leading in themselves and the leading questions that may be asked in cross-examination. The defence strategy is to query the interviewer on their use of leading questions, and yet they use the same method in their questioning of the interviewers. This is subject to criticism by psychologists and sociologists.

Again in R v Ellis [23] Lynda Morgan, Senior Practitioner and Specialist Interviewer gave evidence about the use of leading questions.

In response to defence counsel's question:
Would you also agree that leading questions are extremely dangerous with young children of this age in that they can often suggest the answer to the child?

Lynda Morgan:
When you say that I think there are a number of factors involved, they are very dangerous in the sense of being unwise in any sort of legal or evidential setting because it can be thought that they may influence the response of the child. Children vary in their ability to resist the content of leading questions and because a leading question is used it does not necessarily invalidate the answer but it certainly raises questions as to the reliability of the answer, so one needs to be able to look broadly at the manner in which this particular child answers questions, and if one does that putting it in context, one may well find evidence that the child discards some leading questions but will respond in the affirmative to others, not because they are leading, but because in fact the information in them is correct and in that instance a child is likely to be able to go on with further exploratory questions that are not leading to be able to recall details consistent with the content of the answer to the leading question.




Credibility and Reliability

The reliability of children as witnesses has traditionally been seen as lacking, or even by some commentators, as absent, purely because they are children. They have been said to be consummate liars and manipulators who change these unfortunate characteristics on maturation. This assumption of unreliability appears to extend back to the days of Salem witch trials and before. Once entrenched, some forms of bias linger on:

The studies from the beginning of this century which support this gloomy view of the unreliability of children have now been heavily criticised (Goodman 1984), and ... modern psychological and medical research suggests that children are much more reliable as witnesses then previously thought. However, legal thinking has not always kept apace with the evolution of the social sciences [24] .

Judge Pigot also recognised this in the Report on Child Witnesses, 1990 when he stated:

Courts still prefer to rely on the accumulated wisdom of the past and have not absorbed or applied the fruits of modern research into child psychology.

Psychiatrists and psychologists have shown enormous interest in the area of child psychology, and in particular the area of children's ability as witnesses. Scientific research studies have been carried out on this aspect of presupposed "unreliability" of children and have proven results to the contrary. In fact it would seem that adult witnesses are as prone to be unreliable as children were always said to be. Studies of witnesses' free recall have revealed that children recall less information than do adults, but that they are more accurate in detailing the information they do recall. [25]

There have also been studies showing that there is no clear relationship between age and honesty, and in a situation of prejudice, children are likely to be better witnesses than adults. [26] This of course is relative to the manner in which children have been asked questions. Research has shown that young children's lies were mostly in response to a misdeed, that is, they were reactive to an event that had occurred rather than a fabrication of an event that did not occur.

Hence, we can speculate that if young children were to lie about sexual abuse, it is more likely that they would deny that such abuse occurred than fabricate a false allegation about its occurrence. [27]

Free recall of events in the past of a child in an interview, seldom provides enough disclosure information on which to form Court testimony. [28] Free recall and general questioning elicit more limited responses from children, than do more specific questioning techniques, but interviewers who use the latter are then liable to criticism in that these can be construed as "leading questions". However, there are major differences between correctly phrased specific questions and leading questions, differences which the lay person cannot always easily identify. Spencer & Flin report:

There is little doubt that the quality of a witness's report is dependent on the communication skills of the interviewer. Any specific questioning will increase the likelihood of incorrect responses and the rate of error appears to be directly related to the complexity of the questions posed. But even with simple sentence constructions, the power of language is easily demonstrated, asking "Did you see a knife?" is less suggestive than "Did you see the knife?" and even small children may be responsive to the change from the indefinite to definite article. (Dale et al. 1978). It is well known that leading questions can be particularly hazardous and the risk of suggestibility. [29]

and,

We also know that children are more influenced by leading questions:

(a)            when being asked about descriptions of people or things, rather than events;

(b)           when they are pressed to provide additional details;

(c)            when they do not have a good memory of the information in the question;

(d)           after a long delay;

(e)            when the interview is stressful; and

(f)             when the interviewer lacks appropriate skills.(Dent 1990) [30]

The Court of Appeal in R v Ellis [1994] stated when examining the issue of interviewing processes in the case being flawed:

As the Courts have said in a number of cases, when dealing with young children some coaxing and guidance is necessary to bring them to a point of disclosing abuse which many of them find embarrassing or distasteful and would rather forget. It is unreal to expect them to behave as mature adult witnesses and launch into their evidence with only minimal guidance in examination-in-chief. [31]

Another area of stated unreliability in the child witness is that they, more than adults, are highly suggestible and prone to fantasy. As recently as 1987 in the House of Lords debate on the Criminal Justice Bill, Lord Paget, an English lawyer stated:

Children do not speak the truth naturally. In the normal way children live so much in the world of their imagination. Another point is the tremendous and emotional suggestibility of children. [32]

Spencer and Flin however state that:

These attitudes are derived from cultural and legal mythology. [33]

Further the research of psychologists Gudjonsson and Clark published in 1986 rightly concludes that adults are also liable to be highly suggestible. This study and others (Baxter 1990, Goodman and Reed 1986) point out that children and adults are likely to be influenced by leading questions for a variety of reasons.

Gail Goodman expands on this stating that the issue is more whether children are easily led or suggestible when questioned about personally significant events, such as sexual abuse. Spencer & Flin  [34] set out Goodman's experiment relating to this issue: she interviewed children after they had received routine medical procedures, such as inoculation, giving a blood sample, or a full medical, including a genital examination. When these children, aged three to eight years, were later asked questions about what took place, several suggestive questions of the type that would be asked of a suspected victim were included: "Did he kiss you?" "She took your clothes off first, didn't she?" "Did he ask you to keep a secret about your private parts ?" "How many times did he spank you?" Her results demonstrate that even following delays of a year (reported Goodman 1990) children are extremely unlikely to make false reports of abuse in response to leading questions.

Certainly, given their need for approval, children can be easily swayed. However helpful this knowledge may be in understanding children, it does not assist the interviewers because of the Court's evidential rules regarding leading questions in examination-in-chief and the latitude allowed in cross-examination and re-examination. Until the law is altered to give these interviewers more latitude in the way they interview children in order to elicit the best evidence, they have no option but to be wary of the use of suggestible questions in interview situations.

There are several suggestions for the interviewing of children from research child psychologists: Moston (1987) suggests fewer interviews. Dent (1990) suggests honest ground rules be set by the interviewer stating they know nothing of the child's allegations and that they maintain this openness throughout the interview with no covert assumptions — the child has the lead. King and Yuille (1987) recommend that:

Children need to understand that the interviewer is only interested in what the child remembers and that the admissibility of memory failures and memory gaps are expected. [35]

In conclusion it would appear that children, like adults, are suggestible, and it is the skill, sensitivity and questioning techniques of the interviewer that are essential in eliciting the truth in child witness accounts. Lawyers however are critical of these skills and the truth thus presented in Court and often hone in on this aspect of videotaped evidence in chief whereas:

Paradoxically, they do not seem to be in the least bit concerned about their own use of leading questions in cross-examination and the effects this may have on the quality of the child's evidence. [36]

However Eva Smith, a legal commentator from Scandinavia, states that a child's suggestibility with leading questions can have a positive outcome for that child: [37]

Some of these things are so shameful to the child that it cannot bear to relate them until a grown-up has mentioned them. Leading questions seem a necessity making it difficult to evaluate the credibility of the answer.



Summary

This area of evidential interviews and the qualifications and skills required by the interviewers, is one which fosters much argument from both prosecution and defence. It is an area where it can be difficult to recognise and understand the high level of skill necessary in achieving a legally acceptable interview, whilst at the same time responding to the needs of the child complainant for security and validation. This creates its own dilemmas in terms of child witness credibility. The tribunal of fact has the dilemma of weighing up the child's credibility, with the myths and stereotypes of the society with which they live on the one hand, and on the other the reality of the child's testimony — which tells a very different story; hurt and anguish as opposed to lies, fantasy and distortion. This whole area is one in which the lawyer as sole expert has been shunted aside to allow room for other experts to enter, and this reformation has not been easily accommodated.

If the child can combat the legal system and the evidential interview/s, they then have to sustain their reality in the test of cross examination. In addition, their testimony can be undermined by the cross examination of their care givers, and the interviewers and their techniques being placed under scrutiny in order to ensure that they did not lead the child into false allegations by the use of leading questions.

Legal restrictions and societal attitudes place a burden on both child, interviewer and any parent or care giver who may have inadvertently led the child through the allegations. Psychological research has proven that leading questions are sometimes the most appropriate way to assist a child complainant to verbalise their abuse. They can give permission to speak the unspeakable, as well as help draw more details. The language of a child compared to an adult is still at an evolutionary stage and, like statutes, needs interpretation for their true intent, meaning and spirit to be ascertained — leading questions can assist interpretation.

Until the justice system can allow more flexibility in this area of leading questions at the evidence-in-chief and cross examination level, the child witness is frequently deprived of their rights to justice. I am not advocating an "anything goes" approach, but rather a carefully devised system of ascertaining which questioning techniques and non verbal prompts could be used in each individual case to assist the truth to surface.

This system or process would form part of the standard training programme for interviewers and Judges alike (thus providing uniformity). Uniformity in turn gives consistency which can only aid credibility, and which will also help create safety for children who need clear emotional and physical boundaries if they are to be able to disclose. Perhaps the concept of a child advocate, similar to that used within the Family Court, would be of benefit to children in criminal cases. An advocate to not only explain step by step the process of interviews and cross examination, but also to act as an informed educator to families about what method of communication with the child is "safe" with regards to any issue of contamination that could be raised by either the defence or prosecution.

Every child witness deserves the right to an interview which gives the maximum support possible — not just support in the sense of reducing stress, but support to tell their story in full detail. Only then can the trier of fact adequately decide on their credibility.




Footnotes

[1]      R v H (High Court Wellington T 34/93) Unreported 27th August 1993, 5-6

[2]      R v Seth (CA 105/92) Judgment 26 November 1992, 16

[3]      Report of Inquiry into Child Abuse in Cleveland (Home Office 1987) Chapter 12, 207-8

[4]      Facilitated: where a third person comes into the interview to provide support and clarity of the issues involved.

[5]      Some centres videotape all interviews as a matter of course.

[6]      Greg Walsh, "Re-establishing balance in the criminal justice system" (1991) LSJ 70, 72

[7]      Pipe, Gee & Wilson, ibid 26

[8]      Greg Walsh, ibid 72

[9]      R Gardner Sex Abuse Hysteria, Salem Witch Trials Revisited (1991) 52-3

[10]    Draft Sexual Abuse Guidelines, Department of Social Welfare, Circular Memorandum 1988/171 Pt III

[11]    R v Ellis [1993] 3 NZLR; Transcript trial notes p 409

[12]    Goode, "The Politics of Child Sexual Abuse in the Role of Criminal Law" (1989) CLJ 39

[13]    R v Ellis [1993] supra n.11,419

[14]    Glanville Williams, "Videotaping Children's Evidence" (1987) NLJ 108,111

[15]    Glanville Williams, ibid, 112

[16]    Memorandum of Good Practice on Videorecorded Interviews with Child Witnesses for Criminal Proceedings, Home Office; Department of Health, 1992

[17]    Jenny McEwan (1993) Jnl of Child law 17

[18]    Gerrilyn Smith, "Good Practice or yet another Hurdle: video recording children's statement" (1993), 5 Jnl of Child Law 21

[19]    Gerrilyn Smith ibid, 22

[20]    JC Yuile, Robin Hunter,Risha Joffe & Judy Zaparnuik, University of British Columbia Child Victims, Child Witnesses, Understanding and Improving Testimony, Eds Goodman & Bottoms (1993) The Guilford Press, 99

[21]    JR Spencer & RH Flin, The Evidence of Children: The Law & Psychology, Blackstone Press Ltd (1990), 276

[22]    Spencer & Flin, ibid, 277

[23]    R v Ellis [1993] supra n.11,386

[24]    Spencer & Flin, supra n.21,237

[25]    Gail S Goodman, Christine Aman and Jodi Hirschman, Child Sexual and Physical Abuse: Children's Testimony in Children's Eyewitness Memory (1987 SJ Ceci, MP Toglia and DF Ross, eds, Springer-Verlag) 12

[26]    GB Melton, "Children's Competency to testify" (1981) 5 Law & Human Behaviour 73, 79

[27]    K.Bussey, K.Lee & E.Grimbeek, 'Lies and Secrets: Implications for Children's Reporting of Sexual Abuse', supra n.19 at 153.

[28]    Margaret-Ellen Pipe, Susan Gee, Clare Wilson, 'Cues, Props, and Context : Do They Facilitate Children's Event Reports?' in Child Victims, Child Witnesses, Understanding and Improving Testimony, Eds Goodman & Bottoms, 1993 The Guildford Press, pp25-45

[29]    Spencer & Flin , supra n.21, 241

[30]    Spencer & Flin, supra n.21, 254

[31]    R v Ellis [1994] Court of Appeal CA 274/93 at 10

[32]    Hansard, October 22nd 1987, Volume 489, No 22, column 282

[33]    Spencer and Flin, supra n.21, 253

[34]    Spencer & Flin, supra n.21, 253-4

[35]    Spencer & Flin, supra n.21, 253-4

[36]    Spencer & Flin, supra n.21, 257

[37]    Eva Smith, "How to Deal with a Child's Evidence" (1989) 22 JCL 33.