The Christchurch Civic Creche Case

News Reports Index

2003 Sept



The Press
September 4, 2003.

Perspective

Civic Creche: Who can you really believe?
How valid is the evidence in the Civic Creche trial?

Left, Bernard Robertson suggests an inquiry is needed into the case and questions whether psychologists do more harm than good, while, right, Chris Goddard and Neerosh Mudaly ask why adults choose not to hear what abused children have to say.




Psychologists regularly give opinions which result in families being broken up, people going to jail, people being released from or kept in jail. This is a house of cards and the Ellis case is the card that could bring it all tumbling down.


An inquiry is needed into the case
by Bernard Robertson


Bernard Robertson, editor of The New Zealand Law Journal and co-author of Interpreting Evidence: Evaluating Forensic Science in the Courtroom (John Wiley, UK 1995) and of Cross on Evidence (NZ ed)


Parents know that children are different. Their frame of reference is different, they have little understanding of the consequences of their actions, sometimes they are contrary, sometimes keen to please.

Yet in the early 1990s, the psychological mantra was "children never lie". Even if this is limited to saying that children never deliberately say something they know is untrue, parents know this is not right. Children will lie to stay out of trouble, or to receive a benefit.

The proper point in psychology was that for therapeutic purposes one takes what clients say at face value. One does not say "well that sounds nonsense to me, get a grip of yourself’. But to accept that what the client is saying is also an accurate reflection of reality is a leap of faith which has no justification. But this is precisely what psychologists, and less well-qualified people who call themselves "sex abuse counsellors” or whatever, did in the 1980s and ‘90s.

Sadly, psychologists are notoriously gullible. In controlled studies with prepared scripts, no psychologist ever detected a malingerer. The whole split-personality farce was just a case of clever psychopaths feeding psychologists what they wanted to hear. There has never been a proven instance of split personality but every psychologist wants to find one and publish articles about it.  This gullibility extended itself into fury at judges throwing out psychological evidence about children in court cases. The common law developed various ritual warnings that had to be given to juries in sexual offences and with child witnesses. No doubt these rituals confused juries and resulted in some wrongful acquittals. But in this area of the law as in others it is better that the guilty go free than that the innocent are convicted. It is not clear that psychologists accept that, however, and some of what  they and others are saying is dangerously close to saying that Peter Ellis and others must remain convicted so as to help with the therapy of undoubtedly disturbed people.

In the 1980s, evidence was given in court in sex abuse cases by half-trained counsellors and others which was rightly rejected by the Court of Appeal. It was rejected because it was illogical and did not prove what the witnesses seemed to think it proved. Rather than learn from the experience, the psychological lobby managed to get section 23G of the Evidence Act passed.

This enables psychologists to give evidence about all sorts of ill-defined things. In particular they can give evidence about whether a child’s behaviour is "compatible or incompatible" with sexual abuse.  This is meaningless. There is no behaviour which is incompatible with sexual abuse, so all of us, all the time, exhibit behaviour that is compatible with sexual abuse. "Compatible with" does not mean "evidence for" or "supporting" but  that is how surveys show lawyers,  police, and perhaps jurors understand it.

Despite the latitude which section 23G gives, it seems that psychologists are unable to stay within its bounds. Two recent Court of Appeal cases threw out evidence given by experienced witnesses who have given evidence in many other cases, one of them Dr Karen Zelas, who figured in the Christchurch creche case. Now, appeals represent only the tip of the iceberg.  Many people do not appeal even against convictions because they cannot afford it or because they want to put it behind them and get on with their lives. If an appeal case shows that a prominent psychological witness has misunderstood the boundaries of section 23G, then it seems to me that there should be an inquiry and a re-examination of cases in which that witness gave evidence, in order to ensure that the same errors were not committed in those cases.

This is why there needs to be an inquiry into the Peter Ellis case, and this is the reason why the establishment is vigorously resisting an inquiry. It is not just a matter of the guilt or innocence of Peter Ellis but of (among other things) the whole functioning of the judicial system and the role of psychologists in it.

The Court of Appeal in the 1980s believed that psychology would “grow as a science" and things would improve. Sadly, this will not happen. Psychology is not a science. It has no agreed theoretical foundation and after a century of floundering around has failed to come up with any general theory of human behaviour. The differences between schools of psychology are not about detail, they are about fundamentals.

Nor does psychology "grow". While physical sciences constantly refine and expand their understanding of the world, psychology is swept by new ideas which wipe out everything that went before. When the sex abuse diagnostic manual is revised it is completely rewritten and often contradicts what was in previous editions. Psychology is no nearer a general theory than it was 100 years ago.

Nor is psychology of much practical value. There is no evidence that counselling does any good, and plenty of evidence that it does harm. To the extent that talking to someone can be valuable for some people, no theoretical knowledge is required to make you a good listener. Most psychological problems come from brain chemistry and can be alleviated with drugs, diet, and exercise.  Evidence from controlled studies is that psychologists are no better at predicting future dangerousness than schoolchildren and secretaries.

Yet psychologists regularly give opinions which result in families being broken up, people going to jail, people being released from or kept in jail. This is a house of cards and the Ellis case is the card that could bring it all tumbling down.





Child death inquiries from around the world have repeatedly shown that we do not want to talk to child victims and do not listen when they try to reveal what is happening to them.


Why adults choose not to hear what abused children have to say
by Chris Goddard and Neerosh Mudaly

Dr Chris Goddard is director of the Child Abuse and Family Violence research unit at Monash University, Melbourne. Dr Neerosh Mudaly is senior counsellor with the Australian Childhood Foundation.


Children and young people who have been abused and neglected are rarely heard. It is as if they are beaten into silence, neglected by the community and the State as well as by abusive adults. The reality may be that we do not want to hear what they say, that their words are too painful to absorb. They are silenced by our talk of cases, notifications, investigations, and performance targets.

Child death inquiries from around the world have repeatedly shown that we do not want to talk to child victims and do not listen when they try to reveal what is happening to them.

Victoria Climbie, an eight-year- old West African child, died in London after shocking abuse by her aunt and her aunt's boyfriend. Beaten, starved, and burned, she was wrapped in a bin liner and left in her own excrement in an icy bathroom. Many professionals failed Victoria. After they received reports, police decided there were no crimes to investigate — without speaking to her.

In Australia, Daniel Valeric also had more than 100 bruises. He spoke few words. His older brother, aged four, had a wider vocabulary and could describe what was happening. Sometimes even direct reports from a child are not enough.

We know about these fatal failures only because the media ensured Daniel's and Victoria's voices and silences carried beyond the grave. It is as if only posthumous complaints repeated and reported by journalists are loud enough to be heard.

Crimes against children are committed in hundreds of Australian homes every day. In spite of the mortality and morbidity rates, there is scandalously little research.

How the children who have been assaulted think and feel about their traumatic experiences has remained largely unexplored. Research we have conducted at Monash University suggests children and young people who have been abused can contribute a great deal to our understanding of violence in the home.

One 11-year-old girl understood only too well why she had been sexually abused. She saw herself as the "easiest target". "It's too difficult to go out into a shopping centre . . . and catch whoever comes into the toilet  . . .  I was there at the time . . . I was young and did not know what it was."

A teenager, sexually abused since infancy, thought such assaults were "normal". Only when she started secondary school did she begin to learn that her father's abuse was out of the ordinary.

Children also have clear insights into the causes of abuse. A 12-year- old understood: "They are picking on you because you are smaller than them." It is ". . . the way they've been treated by their parents . . . because they have had it done to them they lash out . . . they don't know any way to channel their anger . . . that's how they've been shown, that's how they've seen it". An 11-year-old boy:". . . he needed help, so he made someone else need help even worse."

Children and young people understand the effects of abuse. "I had to grow up too quickly and I just feel that I missed out on childhood pretty much, which is a pretty huge thing to cope with . . . I really struggle with that . . . you have to grow up very fast in that sort of situation, otherwise you don't survive. It's as simple as that." Another young person described how abuse "just creeps into every part of your life".

A number of children described how the abuse would always be with them. A 13-year-old boy: "I think it will be there really the rest of my life." Children can understand the sense of complete powerlessness. "I didn't have any control over my life . . . so I started controlling my food; I would either eat it and throw it up again, or I wouldn't eat."

Perhaps we would listen more carefully if we realised how difficult it is for the children to tell. A 13-year-old boy: "I did, but I didn't want to tell. I wanted to but didn't want to . . . I was thinking, if I told and he found out, I would be in serious trouble." Another child told why it was hard to disclose: "Probably because some things like that are embarrassing and because some people hadn't believed me." It took some children years to tell: "They threatened me, 'if you don't do this I'll do this to you'."

Fear features strongly in some accounts. One 11-year-old explained what it was like to disclose abuse at the age of eight; in a loud voice he told how he was "frightened, very frightened".

Some children also have remarkable insight into how some adults minimise the abuse of children. A 12-year-old girl, subject to abuse and a witness to domestic violence: "They don't want to believe the truth, they just want to believe the easiest side . . . the simplest basically . . . They don't want to hear the truth because the truth is so much harder to understand . . ."

Our failure to listen closely to children who have been abused contributes to every child's vulnerability. A system that refuses to release essential data - or does not collect it in the first place — does not have child protection as its priority. A system that obstructs or prohibits research aids and abets the abuser rather than the victim - holds up a ghastly mirror to us all.

It reflects a world where it is preferred that victims and their advocates remain silent, where the "simplest" and "easiest" options for adults are chosen, all because the truth is "harder to understand".