Child Sex Abuse Hysteria & The Ellis case

Focus on People - Hall of Fame

Jane Rawls - Index



Lawtalk   New Zealand
452   April, 1996.        Pages 28-29.

How questions and body-parts diagrams could affect the content of young children’s disclosures

(Dr) Jane Mary Rawls
Registered Psychologist, Clinical and Child Psychologist

The primary mode of gathering evidence from children is through interviewing. Report veracity can be tested against physical evidence but in some sexual abuse cases, for example, this may be absent, and we are left merely with the adults' questions and the child's answers.


Judges, lawyers, and juries clearly believe that the age and abilities of the child witness can affect their understanding of legal questions and procedures, and s23 (g) of the Evidence Amendment Act (1989) reflects this.  Despite this, the court process still remains complex and requires children to demonstrate a clear understanding of truth, lies, and promises in a way that might make some adults falter, and to answer complex questions in Court.

The process of interviewing before and during a Court appearance is an interactive and fluid one that is influenced and shaped by the form of the questions asked as well as the information provided by the answers given. There is now a proliferation of child testimony research that looks at the effects of different question forms on the accuracy and malleability of children's reports (for a review see Ceci & Bruck, I993, in the Psychological Bulletin). The NZ Law Foundation recently funded a study by the author that examined the effect of repeated interviews on young children's reports of events in which interviews consisted of either Closed questions such as may be asked of a child by a concerned person (e.g., "Did (s)he touch you on the...?"), or Open questions of the sort that should be asked in an evidential interview (e.g., "What happened?"), or a mixture of these two forms of questions. The believability of these children's reports, as assessed by a panel of professionals working in the legal arena (lawyers, police. psychologists), and the relationship with s23 (g) (Evidence Amendment Act 1989), will be described in a later article.

Thirty 5-year-olds have taken part in the study so far (and seven 4-year-olds are still taking part). Each has participated individually in a series of four videotaped and observed sessions in which a male adult research assistant played a dressing-up game with the child. These sessions involved small amounts of touching when items such as hats and jewellery were put on or taken off each other, and sometimes required the child to keep secret an additional minor benign event. Four interviews were conduced over a period 1 and 2 weeks after the games, and included a segment in which the child was asked about truth, lies, and promises, before being asked to indicate who touched whom, where the touching occurred, and with what part of the toucher's body.  A body-parts diagram (similar to those used in evidential interviews) was introduced in the second interview as a prop to facilitate reports. Interviews were conducted with 10 children in each of either Open (who, where, when, what), Closed (yes or no answer), or Mixed (both open and closed) question conditions.

Only 40% of the 5-year-old sample could, after varying degrees of exposure to examples, provide an acceptable definition of truth, lies, and promises.  Half of these children happened to have been in the closed questioning interview condition, whose participants subsequently produced the least accurate reports of interactions and the most fantasy.  Thus, it appears that the question form used in the interview was a more accurate predictor of report accuracy than promising to tell the truth.

All the children could describe in general terms having had several dress-up games with a man (but varied widely over the number of sessions) and could name many of the dress-up items they had played with and on which body-part they had been worn.  But they had much more difficulty saying when these events had occurred historically (varying from "yesterday" to "5 months ago") and/or allocating specific dress-up events to the correct session.  Results showed the initial superiority of open questions in obtaining accurate reports.  For example, when children were interviewed for the first time about the first dress-up session, open questions resulted in an average accuracy of 32% correct compared with those interviewed with mixed questions (20%) or closed questions (9%).  Similarly, in this first interview, children's descriptions of the last dress-up session were more accurate when open questions were used (13%), less accurate when interviews comprised mixed questions (4%) and not at all accurate (0%/) when closed questions were used.  However, by the fourth time they were questioned about the first session, the accuracy of children in the closed questions had increased (16%) as had those asked mixed questions (25%), but had decreased for the children being asked open questions (I5%). Other research (cf Ceci & Bruck, 1993) has shown that while direct (closed) questions can serve as a prompt for memory recall, they may also lead to intrusion of other inaccuracies because they depend on the options provided by the interviewer.

Provisional analyses of data indicate that children found it easier to recount the items used during dress-up games (32% accurate) and who had chosen and received (30% each) specific items, and on which part of the body they had been worn (28%), but they found it extremely difficult to provide an accurate account of the sequence in which dress-up items had been chosen and placed on their own and the adult’s body (4% accuracy).

Of concern was the frequency and type of commission errors made by those children interviewed with closed questions and, to a lesser degree, those asked a mix of open and closed questions.  Their errors seemed to evolve over time with repeated interviews and, for many, they first were reported when body-parts diagrams were used during their second interviews.  For some children, these errors seemed relatively harmless and included climbing ladders, going to other rooms, having other children present, wearing elaborate costumes, and tickling with feathers.  But the elaborations of other children were more serious. Nearly a quarter of the total sample of children (24%) reported inappropriate adult-child touching. Three children (10%) reported genital touching (two under closed questioning and one under mixed questioning) that involved either adult-child, child-adult, or mutual touching, and two of these children also reported touching under their upper clothes.  Two more children (7%) reported either the adult touching their bottom or their touching the adult's bottom and two others (7%) reported mutual adult-child touching under their clothing (e.g., rubbing in pretend cream).  Reports of mutual undressing without touching were also common, although this often seemed to reflect a confusion between dress-up items and ordinary clothes.  The videotaped record of dress-up interactions did not support these reports.  Each child was debriefed for ethical reasons at the end of the series of interviews.

The accuracy of children's diagram markings used to illustrate adult-child and child-adult touching was compared with the videotaped records of actual mutual touching.  Again, children in the open question condition clearly were more accurate about adult-child touching when presented with body-parts diagrams (71% and 60%, for adult to child and child to adult respectively) than children asked mixed questions (31% and 38%) or those asked closed questions (28% and 22%).  This interactive effect of the body-parts diagrams with the type of question form used during interviews was unprogrammed and unexpected, but it clearly demonstrates that open questions used alone resulted in the most accurate reports, whilst the mixed and closed questioning resulted in significantly reduced report accuracy.

"Secrets" were also programmed for each child. Children never volunteered to tell them but, when specifically asked about them, seven (23%) always declined to disclose, eight (27%) sometimes described them accurately and sometimes didn’t disclose, six (20%) consistently provided accurate accounts, three (10%) gave some true and some false accounts, and one (3%) either gave no account or a false one. False accounts were always benign and seemed to be given to avoid breaking the child's implicit contract with the adult assistant.  Five other children (17%) described fictional unprogrammed events that included inappropriate touching and said that they were the programmed "secrets".

In summary, it seems that the use of interviews consisting of open-ended questions only is by far the safest option when interviewing children as it results in greater accuracy of reports after a minimal delay and, overall, decreases the chances of commission errors, especially when body-parts diagrams are used as an integral part of that interview. The implications of these results for future complainants, and their relevance to evidential and court procedures, were the focus of a series of seminars planned for members of the legal profession in late March. Further seminars are proposed for later in the year.



Biographical note:

Dr Jane Rawls completed a Masters degree in Psychology and a post-graduate Diploma in Clinical Psychology at the University of Waikato and a PhD in Developmental and Child Psychology at the University of Kansas in the USA.  She has a private practice as a child and clinical psychologist in Hamilton, serving as a Specialist Report Writer for the Family Court and consulting as an expert witness in the High Court.  Her doctoral research was in the area of children's testimony and she has continued to research in this area, supported in past years by research grants from FRST.  Over 1995 she has worked on the research described in this article, supported by a research grant from the NZ Law Foundation.